Close

Kin structure and roost fidelity in greater noctule bats

João D. Santos, Christoph F.J. Meyer, Carlos Ibáñez, Ana G. Popa-Lisseanu, Javier Juste

Preprint posted on 27 February 2020 https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/675215v2

Article now published in Basic and Applied Ecology at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.baae.2021.02.001

Home is where mom is: Santos et al find female greater noctule bats share their maternity roosts more with their mothers and daughters. Explores how genetic relatedness or natal philopatry (faithfulness to birthplace) influence roost sharing in bats

Selected by Baheerathan Murugavel

Background

Many mammals, including bats, are known to exhibit ‘natal philopatry’ – a trait where individuals return to their birthplace during their breeding seasons (Greenwood, 1980). Temperate bats exhibit increased female philopatry whose genetic signature can be traced back even at the mitochondrial level. Studies focussing on the roosting behavior of maternity colonies have reported the existence of physically defined social groups within colonies in the bat species Nyctalus lasiopterus (Popa- Lisseanu et al., 2008). However, not much is known about whether genetically related individuals share the same social group and prefer to roost together. In this study, using microsatellite markers, the authors have verified how genetic relatedness of individuals influences roost fidelity in social groups of N. lasiopterus in a maternity colony whose roosting behavior has been well documented by field observations and radio-tracking in the past (Popa- Lisseanu et al., 2008). The results of the current study contribute to the understanding of whether roost sharing is dictated through genetic relatedness or natal philopatry in female bats. The authors have also discussed why natal philopatry could be more advantageous in choosing roost mates than genetic relatedness in such maternity colonies.

Key findings

In this preprint, the authors showed that mother-daughter pairs share the same social group more frequently than expected and that the levels of group-sharing increases with the levels of genetic relatedness within the group members. In other words, bats in the predefined social groups shared their roosts with their mothers, daughters, or at least one of their close genetic relatives. Finally, the authors genotyped individuals that were previously radio-tracked who had shared the same roost and had found that roost associations among them are not kin-based.

Since this study colony is an already well-studied population in the context of roosting behavior (Popa- Lisseanu et al., 2008), the authors used the previously defined subdivisions/social groups, and assessed the genetic structure of the whole population after which they made comparisons among those groups. Using 11 microsatellite markers, they have carefully estimated both pairwise and the mean relatedness among individuals and found that 81% of the individuals shared their social group with at least one close relative.

In order to test whether the relationship status among individuals influences group sharing, the authors grouped pairs into one of the four defined relations, namely Parent- Offspring (PO), Full-Sibling (FS), Half-Sibling (HS), or Unrelated (U), based on pairwise relatedness estimates. In comparison, they found that proportions of only the parent-offspring (PO) pairs in the social groups differed significantly from a random expectation indicating mother-daughter pairs shared the same social group more frequently than expected

Using the same categories and pairwise relationship estimates, they also found that the proportions of individuals sharing the same group increased as the proportion of relatedness among them increased, indicating that these bats tend to share their roosts with their genetic relatives.

In addition, to check whether roost associations are kin-based or not, the authors have genotyped the same radio-tracked individuals from the earlier study and had tested the relationship between roost association and genetic relatedness in this subset. They did not find any significant association between roost use and genetic relatedness in this subset, which shows that roost associations are less likely to be kin-based and more likely to be due to philopatry.

The authors discuss why philopatry could be more useful for females when it comes to choosing roosting partners. They cite and hypothesize that the young adult females could benefit from their mothers’ experiences in choosing a roost during such seasons (associative learning) or could establish social bonding with their mothers leading to benefits of co-operative behaviors such as information transfer and mutually nursing each others’ young ones (sociality). However, the authors have clearly stated that these hypotheses are based on certain assumptions from the available information on the roosting and social behavior of this species. More behavioral observation is needed to support these hypotheses and to better understand the benefits of natal philopatry.

Why I like this preprint?

Bats, being the only flying mammal, have certain unique challenges when it comes to breeding and nursing the young (Kerth et al., 2008). I liked this study because the authors have explored the genetic structures of previously known social groups in a well-studied bat colony and have tried to explore the influence of natal philopatry on the social structuring of these bats. In order to understand concepts like natal philopatry and their relationship with roosting, such studies that follow up existing behavioral knowledge and test their molecular basis are very crucial. Without integrating field-based knowledge on roosting behavior with such carefully designed follow up studies like this, it would be otherwise impossible to understand such concepts in free-ranging mammals such as bats.

 

Future directions/questions to the authors

  1. The authors have mentioned the years in which they had collected the samples but nothing has been mentioned about the sexual status of the individuals during the sampling. Were all the sampled individuals in the same status (pregnant/ lactating during the sampling?
  2. Will the roost associations change with the status of a breeding female? Is the nonsignificance in roost associations and genetic relatedness consistent throughout the breeding season?
  3. How many individuals were there in each of the social groups studied? Is the high within-group relatedness an artefact of just the group size? Can the authors comment on the existence of any threshold group size after which individuals might start to disperse?
  4. In extension with the above question, was group II having more individuals than groups I and III and hence did not have significantly different mean relatedness with the total? Could it be that as the number of individuals increases in the social group, the relatedness gets low?
  5. How do the authors propose to promote conservation of the species with the findings of this study?

References

Greenwood, P.J. (1980). Mating systems, philopatry, and dispersal in birds and mammals. Anim. Behav. 28, 1140–1162.

Kerth, G. (2008). Causes and consequences of sociality in bats. Bioscience 58, 737.

Popa-Lisseanu, A.G., Bontadina, F., Mora, O. & Ibáñez, C. (2008). Highly structured fission-fusion societies in an aerial-hawking, carnivorous bat. Anim. Behav. 75, 471–482.

 

Tags: bats, natal philopatry

Posted on: 22 April 2020

doi: https://doi.org/10.1242/prelights.19100

Read preprint (2 votes)

Have your say

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

Sign up to customise the site to your preferences and to receive alerts

Register here
Close